Cornell Chronicle index page Table of Contents Front page of this issue

Brain-damaged patients may be aware of surroundings, research suggests

Nicholas Schiff, assistant professor of neurology and neuroscience and director of the Laboratory of Cognitive Neuromodulation at Weill Cornell Medical Center. He also is assistant attending neurologist at NewYork-Presbyterian Hospital/Weill Cornell Medical Center. Amelia Panico

By Ernie Mundell

For the first time, advanced neurological imaging suggests that the brains of minimally conscious patients recognize and respond to speech in ways similar to healthy individuals.

According to a team of researchers from Columbia University Medical Center and Weill Cornell Medical College, both in New York City, and the JFK Johnson Rehabilitation Institute-Center for Head Injuries in Edison, N.J., the minimally conscious state is distinct from either a persistent vegetative state or coma in that patients show intermittent signs of awareness and may even attempt to communicate using simple words or signals.

"The results challenge our thinking about the possible inner lives these severely brain-damaged patients may experience, and also motivate renewed interest in research aimed at recovery and rehabilitation," said the study's senior author, Joy Hirsch, professor of neuroradiology and psychology, and director of the fMRI (functional magnetic resonance imaging) Research Center at Columbia Medical Center.

The study was published in the Feb. 8 issue of Neurology.

Unfortunately, episodes of awareness in minimally conscious patients are erratic, "so that most of the time caregivers and loved ones are left unable to make contact with the patient," explained study lead author Nicholas D. Schiff, assistant professor of neurology and neuroscience and director of the Laboratory of Cognitive Neuromodulation at Weill Cornell. He also is assistant attending neurologist at NewYork-Presbyterian Hospital/Weill Cornell Medical Center.

"In fact, many observers remain doubtful whether minimally conscious patients are even aware of the external world," he said.

To help shed light on that mystery, Schiff turned to the state-of-the-art fMRI at Hirsch's Columbia lab. These studies, which have potentially broad implications for rehabilitation, arose from ongoing collaborations between Schiff and Joseph T. Giacino, associate director of neuropsychology at the JFK Johnson institute.

Using fMRI -- which employs MRI scanning technology to track real-time brain function -- the team compared neurological activity in seven healthy volunteers with that of two minimally conscious patients, both young men. One of the minimally conscious subjects had experienced sudden brain hemorrhage, while the other's injuries were caused by blunt trauma to the head.

During the experiment, study subjects were exposed to a recording of family members reading a simple narrative aloud while researchers observed brain activity via fMRI.

"The results were a big surprise," Schiff said. "The spoken narratives appeared to activate language networks across many different regions of the brain, in both normal and brain-damaged subjects. In fact, in at least one of the minimally conscious patients we studied, all of the components that were active in healthy subjects were also active in his brain."

A second finding -- which the researchers described as "haunting" -- suggests an even more personal response to speech on the part of minimally conscious subjects. "Especially in one of the two patients, the spoken narratives appeared to activate visual areas of the brain," Hirsch said. "This happens in healthy subjects whenever language evokes visual images."

In a less encouraging finding, the researchers say the "resting activity rate" of the brains of minimally conscious patients remains very low, suggesting they lack the metabolic resources to keep specific cognitive networks running at a consistently high level. The finding could help explain the intermittent, episodic nature of awareness and communication seen in minimally conscious patients.

"Obviously, we still need to find out so much more about the neurological potential of these patients, so research building on these findings is already under way," Schiff said. Technology is at the very heart of this research, Hirsch added. "Brain imaging can be thought of as a way of giving a real voice to the minimally conscious patient," she explained. "It's allowing us as physicians and scientists to be more aware of these patients' potential for rehabilitation."

Giacino noted that while the research is "exciting news," it is too early to speculate as to therapies. "Those of us working in the rehabilitation sphere now have real evidence these severely damaged brains retain essential networks," he said. "These studies should encourage further efforts at rehabilitation to try and break these patients out of their isolation, to better understand what their capacity to experience -- and interact with -- their environment really is," he added.

The research was supported with funding from the National Cancer Institute, the Charles A. Dana Foundation, the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, and the National Institutes of Health-supported NewYork-Presbyterian/Weill Cornell General Clinical Research Center.

Co-researchers include Fred Plum and A. Kamal of Weill Cornell Medical College; Diana Rodriguez-Moreno of Weill Cornell's Graduate School of Medical Sciences; and K.H.S. Kim of Georgetown University Medical Center, Washington, D.C.

Ernie Mundell is a writer at Weill Cornell.

March 3, 2005

| Cornell Chronicle Front Page | | Table of Contents | | Cornell News Service Home Page |