The Architectural Colour Design Process: An Evaluation of Sequential Media via Semantic Ratings

Article (PDF Available)inColor Research & Application 35(5):343 - 351 · October 2010with 72 Reads
DOI: 10.1002/col.20583
Abstract
In recent studies, contextual situations of applied colours are compared to colours presented as samples or chips. Findings of such studies point out different results in terms of similarities or differences between the evaluations of isolated abstract colours and contextualized situations. Architectural and spatial contexts have their own characteristics regarding colouring criteria, so it is of great importance to examine the architectural spatial colouring process from this point of view. This study explores this process by investigating the consistency of semantic ratings of four sequential stages of the architectural colour design process, namely, colour chips samples, abstract compositions, perspective drawings and 3D models. The architectural context for the study was a simple interior space. Fifteen different colour schemes were applied on the four media representing the stages. Subjects rated the 15 sets against seven bipolar, five-step semantic differential scales. The scales consisted of harmonious-discord, pleasant-unpleasant, comfortableuncomfortable, spacious-confined, static-dynamic, exciting-calming and extroverted-introverted. Findings indicated that there are significant associations between the evaluations of the abstract compositions, the perspective drawings and the 3D models; however, the evaluations of colour chips are significantly different than the others. The medium effect observed mostly between abstract and contextualized media. Additionally, factor analysis showed that pleasantness, harmony, spaciousness and comfort are connected in the evaluations of contextual situations, while pleasantness and harmony differ from spaciousness and comfort in the evaluations of colour chips and abstract compositions. The factor of activity (arousal) (dynamism, excitement, and extroversion) stays the same for all four media. It is also found that different colour characteristics are determinative over different media.
The Architectural Colour Design Process:
An Evaluation of Sequential Media via
Semantic Ratings
Sibel Ertez Ural,*Semiha Yilmazer
Department of Interior Architecture and Environmental Design, Faculty of Art, Design and Architecture, Bilkent University, Ankara 06800, Turkey
Received 11 May 2008; revised 21 April 2009; accepted 10 May 2009
Abstract: In recent studies, contextual situations of
applied colours are compared to colours presented as
samples or chips. Findings of such studies point out dif-
ferent results in terms of similarities or differences
between the evaluations of isolated/abstract colours and
contextualized situations. Architectural and spatial con-
texts have their own characteristics regarding colouring
criteria, so it is of great importance to examine the archi-
tectural/spatial colouring process from this point of view.
This study explores this process by investigating the con-
sistency of semantic ratings of four sequential stages of
the architectural colour design process, namely, colour
chips/samples, abstract compositions, perspective draw-
ings and 3D models. The architectural context for the
study was a simple interior space. Fifteen different colour
schemes were applied on the four media representing the
stages. Subjects rated the 15 sets against seven bipolar,
five-step semantic differential scales. The scales consisted
of harmonious-discord, pleasant-unpleasant, comfortable-
uncomfortable, spacious-confined, static-dynamic, excit-
ing-calming and extroverted-introverted. Findings indi-
cated that there are significant associations between the
evaluations of the abstract compositions, the perspective
drawings and the 3D models; however, the evaluations of
colour chips are significantly different than the others.
The medium effect observed mostly between abstract and
contextualized media. Additionally, factor analysis showed
that pleasantness, harmony, spaciousness and comfort are
connected in the evaluations of contextual situations,
while pleasantness and harmony differ from spaciousness
and comfort in the evaluations of colour chips and
abstract compositions. The factor of activity (arousal)
(dynamism, excitement, and extroversion) stays the same
for all four media. It is also found that different colour
characteristics are determinative over different media.
Ó2010 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 35, 343 351, 2010;
Published online 22 January 2010 in Wiley InterScience (www.
interscience.wiley.com). DOI 10.1002/col.20583
Key words: architectural colour design process; spatial
colour; semantic ratings; architectural/spatial context;
colour emotions
INTRODUCTION
In the field of architecture, colour is a design element. It
stands as a composition of all its attributes and it exists at
a place where all related disciplines overlap. As Green-
Armytage says, ‘‘successful colour design depends on the
designer’s knowledge comes from several disciplines and
from design itself.’
1
Theoretical knowledge comes from
several disciplines and arises from research mostly where
colours are abstracted and simplified into colour chips
and samples. The need for reconsideration of the theoreti-
cal knowledge within the architectural context and real-
life situations is inevitable.
In design itself, simplifications and abstractions are
widely used both in design education and by professio-
nals. Choosing isolated colour samples from various cata-
logues, developing abstract colour graphics for initial col-
our decisions and then elaborating on these decisions
through various drawings and models seem to be the most
common approach to the colour design process.
2–4
Each
of these visualization techniques is progressively used by
the participants in the design as a communication medium
throughout the process. Hence, corroboration between the
different visualization techniques is essential to a reliable
colour design method. The presented study deals with dif-
ferent visualization techniques, commonly used during the
architectural colour design process, by investigating the
consistency of their evaluations via semantic ratings.
*Correspondence to: Sibel Ertez Ural (e-mail: ural@bilkent.edu.tr).
V
V
C2010 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Volume 35, Number 5, October 2010 343
The Colour Design Process in Architecture
Colour is an element of architecture, thus colour design
have to be an integral part of architectural design process.
Smith emphasizes that environmental colour is multifac-
eted, playing a variety of roles in our everyday lives.
5
However, prototypes and models for designs are often
presented in white or monochromatic combinations, irre-
spective of the materials incorporated and the colours that
may be applied in the final constructed building, interior
or object. Questions are therefore raised concerning
design professionals’ perceptions of the importance of
colour in relation to space and form, and to the experi-
ence of place. According to Mikellides, an architect wants
or considers it desirable to use colour in his design on the
one hand, but fears or is ignorant of its possibly undesir-
able effects on people on the other hand.
6
Mikellides
explains this conflict as an architectural dilemma: what
we know about colour as scientists and psychologists and
what we do as artists and architects. This conflict can
only be overcome in architecture by bridging colour
theory and application.
This topic continues to be discussed not only in practice
but also in education. Janssens and Mikellides investigated
architectural students’ knowledge about the perceptual and
psycho-physiological aspects of colour, colour nomencla-
ture, existing myths and beliefs and how colour is used in
their everyday work in the studio.
7
The findings show there
is a severe lack of knowledge about colour research. Read-
ing colour literature and attending lectures or conferences
was rare. However, attitudes toward colour research were
positive, especially for studies on colour perception and
colour preferences. Students perceived colour design as
their own future responsibility and basic design education
seemed to be the main source of colour information.
Although this study is over 10 years old, it is likely that
the architectural education has not changed much. Accord-
ing to Akbay,
8
during the progression of colour education
in basic design courses, students’ original colour decision
tendencies—toward subjective and intuitive attitudes—
decreased and their tendencies towards knowledge-based
and analytical attitudes increased.
On the other hand, design students are asking for the
guidance of a methodology in their colour design, and
they need clarification of the ambiguity and generality of
the findings of emperical research. At this point, it is im-
portant to investigate how to implement theoretical colour
knowledge planted via abstract compositions in basic
design into the architectural design process. The product
of design is coloured. The aim of the designer is to end
up with a satisfactory design product supported by a suc-
cessful colour scheme. Particularly in architecture, aes-
thetical and functional criteria are determinative over
evaluating colour design. The colour design is expected to
be compatible with functional appropriateness and
requirements, visual comfort and satisfaction of users’
needs for sure, nevertheless colour harmony and colour
emotions are seemingly the points of departure for achiev-
ing a harmonious, pleasing result, spatial quality and
desired mood. However, the link between harmony
(which is a measure of aesthetics) and pleasantness
(which is a measure of emotion) of colour combinations
is still being discussed.
9
Colour Harmony. In architecture, colour is not an iso-
lated entity; it always exists with other colours of the
form or the space. Thus, harmony of colour combinations
is a major concern for colour designers. On condition that
harmony is a ‘‘measure of aesthetics’’, colour harmony is
tried to be explained on the order of colours.
According to Arnheim,
10
the first attempt to define har-
monic colour combinations dates back to 1839, to Chev-
reul’s statement about harmony of similarities and har-
mony of contrasts. The 20th century has seen several
attempts at explain colour harmony.
11–14
Although these
are not guaranteed formulae, they can be considered clues
to understanding which colours work well together. These
general rules are mostly known as colour schemes, and
are broadly based on Chevreul’s early statement on colour
harmony. According to Burchett,
15
no acceptable model
exists for explaining the concept of colour harmony and
the author identifies key attributes and related terms con-
sistently used to describe it.
In recent years, studies of the dynamics of colour combi-
nations and colour harmony have been done using different
methods. Ha
˚rd and Sivik
16
developed a theoretical model
for colour combinations described on the Natural Color
System. Nemcsics
17
examined the harmony content of
scales found in different locations in various positions of
the axial sections of the Coloroid colour system. U
¨nver and
O
¨ztu
¨rk
4
used contrast quantity limits calculated by the ele-
ments of the Munsell Color System. Ou and Lou
18
devel-
oped a quantitative model for two-colour harmony. Ural
19
examined colour harmony in terms of the colour dynamics
of two-colour combinations by analysing the relations
between three dimensions of colour. However, there is still
no common and generally accepted method for determining
the dynamics of colour combinations.
Colour Emotions. Literature on colour emotions has
been authored mostly by experimental psychologists. The
method of semantic differential introduced by Osgood
et al.
20
is the generally accepted method for colour emo-
tion research. In this method, results are interpreted from
ratings on semantic scales and categories devised by fac-
tor analysis. Lack of control of the stimulus material and
the viewing conditions, and using isolated colour chips
are two points of criticism for such research. From this
point of view findings of studies investigating the consis-
tencies and/or contradictions between evaluations of
abstract/isolated and contextual situations are of great im-
portance. Such investigations are also carried out by com-
paring the profiles of semantic ratings and/or the extracted
factors obtained from different data sets.
The studies investigating the consistencies and/or con-
tradictions between evaluations of abstract/isolated and
contextual situations are compared in detail by Taft,
21
who states that some of the research supports that colour
is an attribute of an object, preferences of colours are
344 COLOR research and application
linked to the objects and colour chip preferences are not
generalizable (e.g., Norman and Scott; Wise and Wise;
Whitfield and Wiltshire; and Davidoff, cited in Taft
21
)
The findings of research which looks at colour-meaning
associations in a wider sense rather than merely at prefer-
ence, point out consistencies between abstract/isolated and
contextual situations to some extent. Osgood et al.
20
found that potency and activity ratings are largely unaf-
fected by the object, while evaluations are dependent on
the object. Taft compared semantic ratings of colour sam-
ples (chips) with those same colours applied to a variety
of familiar objects.
18
Analyses performed on the data
indicated that generally few significant differences existed
between chip and object ratings for the same colour. The
results of this study have implications for the use of col-
our chips in colour planning, as colour chips may serve
as an economical and expedient medium. In Sivik’s
22
research comparing colour chip ratings with colour ratings
of two types of simulated building exteriors, descriptive
scales are largely unaffected by context, while evaluative
scales are most affected by context. In research by Ural,
19
a subject group of architects first generated two-colour
combinations in an abstract medium and then, after apply-
ing those combinations to architecturally defined contexts,
subjects were asked to re-evaluate the new situations.
Findings showed that subjects generally agreed that the
colour combinations had to be reconsidered and they pro-
posed significant colour changes for the new conditions.
The findings also showed that contextual characteristics
are important components of the decision-making process
in architectural colour design. Nevertheless, in an archi-
tectural context, space has its own characteristics which
are quite different from those that an object has. Billger’s
article dealt with the way colours in a room affect each
other through contrast effects and reflections.
23
The men-
tioned study was based on the visual observations of col-
ours in a sequence of experimental situations. It was
found that the effects of both contrasts and reflections
were evident in the spatial appearance of colours.
In recent research, the use of colour chips has also been
examined by making comparisons with the spatial con-
text.
24–26
Destefani and Whitfield questioned how people
select colours for real settings and how they make such
aesthetic decisions.
24
According to the authors, ‘‘after more
than a century of empirical research in psychology, answers
to such elementary question remain elusive and the main
reason is that psychology has focused upon questions of
theoretical interest within psychology, and used colour as a
means to this end. In doing so, typically researchers have
investigated people’s responses to more abstract stimuli—
mainly colour chips—and neglected colour selection for
real-world objects.’’ When Hogg et al.
25
compared seman-
tic ratings of colour samples and a simulated interior space,
they found generally high correlations. However, Stahre
et al.
26
compared the results of a colour-chip study to one
of a full-scale room, and found that the colours were per-
ceived as more distinct and stronger and that they aroused
much stronger emotions in the full-scale room.
Problems and Aims
The literature review reveals the fact that theoretical
knowledge about colour harmony and colour emotions is
based mostly on isolated colour samples and abstract
combinations, and it seems that such knowledge needs to
be reconsidered within an architectural context. Addition-
ally, during the architectural colouring process, colour
chips/samples are widely used at least for initial colour
decisions. Research investigating the consistency of col-
our evaluations in isolated/abstract media and contextual
situations in the field of architecture has great importance
both for knowledge implementation in the field and for
developing architectural colouring methods. Although
there is supplementary research that sheds some light on
the question, any research investigating the consistency of
colour decisions throughout all stages—from isolated
chips to abstract media and finally to the form/space sim-
ulation(s)—has not been found in the literature.
The aim of this study is thus to analyze the assembly
of the stages of the colour design process within an archi-
tectural context and to assist and sustain designers/archi-
tects by sparking a discussion on colour design method.
The scope of the study is circumscribed as comparing the
different kinds of visualizations with each other, rather
than comparing them with real-life situations. Therefore,
this research was designed to explore:
1. Whether the evaluations (semantic ratings) of the col-
our schemes have associations or differences when the
medium changes from colour chips to abstract compo-
sitions, from abstractions to contextual situations and
from 2D to 3D media,
2. Which constructs (semantic scales) go together and how
these constructs are categorized (factorized) when the me-
dium that the colour scheme is presented in differs, and
3. Whether the same colour and scheme characteristics
predict the evaluations (semantic ratings) of the colour
schemes when presented in different media.
METHODS
To investigate the above research questions, the method of
semantic differential was used. In the questionnaire, seven
bipolar five-step scaled adjective pairs were applied. For
investigating constructs related to an architectural context,
evaluative adjectives were preferred and descriptive adjec-
tives were not found to be relevant to the scope and objec-
tives of the research. The chosen adjective pairs were cate-
gorized under the three primary factors of Osgood et al.
20
;
evaluation, activity and potency (pleasure, arousal and
dominance in Mehrabian).
27
The list of adjectives and the
related factors are given in Table I.
Coloured materials used for this research were: colour
schemes presented as colour chips, abstract compositions,
perspective drawings and 3D models—the sequential tools
of colour-design process. These four media were prepared
for a simple architectural space (4 m 35m33m)
assumed to have a study/work function.
Volume 35, Number 5, October 2010 345
Colour chips were prepared as 3 cm 33 cm samples
[Fig. 1(a)]. Abstract compositions were generated as a 9
by 9 checkerboard pattern of 3 cm 33 cm squares, con-
sidering both proportional relations and the adjacency of
coloured areas [Fig. 1(b)]. Perspectives were drawn as
one point on A4 drawing paper [Fig. 1(c)]. In order not to
cause value differences that might affect the appearance
of colour schemes, light, shade and shadow effects were
eliminated in the perspective drawings. 3D models were
produced at a scale of 1/20 [Fig. 1(d)]. For homogeneous
illumination and in order to prevent the effects of glare,
shade and shadow daylight from the north was the pre-
ferred light source.
In an architectural context and particularly in spatial
design, the number of colours combined in a scheme dif-
fers depending on various factors. In this research, the
visual differentiation of surfaces was taken into considera-
tion and the number of colours limited to five. Colours
were chosen for the ceiling, floor, back and left walls,
right wall and the furniture. In the research, 15 different
colour schemes were tested against semantic scales.
Scheme characteristics of colour combinations were
defined by the three variables of the Munsell Color Sys-
tem (Hue, Value, Chroma). Maximum contrast quantity
coefficients
4
were calculated; coefficients under 0.34 were
defined as ‘‘similar,’’ coefficients over 0.66 defined as
‘contrasting’’ and coefficients between the two defined as
‘moderate.’’ For the cases defined as ‘‘similar,’’ the domi-
nant descriptive characteristics of colours were stated as
follows: ‘‘warm’’ or ‘‘cold’’ for Hue, ‘‘light’’ or ‘‘dark’
for Value, ‘‘weak’’ or ‘‘strong’’ for Chroma. For the cases
defined as ‘‘contrasting’’ and ‘‘moderate,’’ the descriptive
quality was stated as ‘‘both’’ for data analyses. Corre-
sponding Munsell notations, schemes and the dominant
descriptive characteristics of the colour combinations can
be seen in Table II.
Subjects consisted of 40 second- and third-year interior
architecture students, who were asked to evaluate 15 col-
our schemes presented in four different media. First, sub-
jects were informed about the given space and assumed
function, then materials were presented on separate desks
and their order changed after each five subjects. In the
experiment room, only indirect daylight (from the north)
was allowed as illumination. The same viewing and illu-
mination conditions of coloured materials were provided
during the experiment. Data consisted of subject ratings
on seven bipolar, five-step semantic differential scales of
15 colour schemes, each presented in four different media
and colour-scheme characteristics (similarity or contrast
and dominant descriptive characteristics over three colour
dimensions). Each subject rated a total of 60 cases against
all scales. The collected data were analysed by SPSS 8.0
for Windows.
RESULTS
This research aimed to examine the effects of context
throughout the architectural colour-design process by
means of different media that, represented the sequential
steps of the process. Data analysis based on the three
research questions is as follows:
i. To see whether there are any associations between
the evaluations of colour schemes presented as chips,
abstract colour graphics, perspective drawings and mod-
els, data sets were analyzed by correlating the semantic
ratings of each medium. Table III shows the correlations
(Pearson’s rand P) between colour chips, abstract
graphics, perspective drawings and 3D models over seven
semantic scales.
Results showed that there is always a significant posi-
tive relationship between semantic ratings of three of the
media—abstract colour graphics, perspective drawings
and models. However, this cannot be said in every case
for colour chips. Colour chips showed significant associa-
tions with all the other media only for the adjective pairs
harmonious/discord and exciting/calming. On the con-
trary, there is no significant association between chips and
TABLE I. Bipolar adjectives used in the questionnaire
and the related factors identified by Osgood et al.
17
Adjectives Factors
a1 Harmonious/discord Evaluation
a2 Pleasant/unpleasant Evaluation
a3 Comfortable/uncomfortable Evaluation
a4 Spacious/confined Potency
a5 Static/dynamic Potency þactivity
a6 Exciting/calming Activity
a7 Introverted/extroverted Activity
FIG. 1. An example of the coloured materials: Colour chips (a), 2D abstract compositions (b), perspective drawings (c),
and 3D models (d).
346 COLOR research and application
all the other media for the adjective pair pleasing/unpleas-
ing. To investigate medium effect on the semantic ratings
of the four media, multiple comparisons in ANOVA
(analysis of variance) were performed. Table IV shows
the medium effect on the semantic ratings. Results
showed an overall significant effect for the type of me-
dium for six of the adjectives. Only for one pair of adjec-
tives (exciting/calming) was no significant effect of me-
dium observed. In Table IV, Scheffe’s range shows where
the medium effect appears.
Scheffe’s range test found that the evaluations of the
colour chips differed from the abstract graphics only for
the adjective pair introverted/extroverted (P¼0.045).
Evaluation of the colour chips differed from the perspec-
tive drawings for the adjective pairs pleasing/unpleasing
(P¼0.013), spacious/confined (P¼0.000) and comforta-
ble/uncomfortable (P¼0.000). Evaluation of the colour
chips differed from the model for the adjective pairs com-
fortable/uncomfortable (P¼0.002) and static/dynamic (P
¼0.008). Semantic ratings of the abstract graphics dif-
fered from the perspective drawings for the adjective pairs
spacious/confined (P¼0.000) and introverted/extroverted
(P¼0.000). They differed from the model for the adjec-
tive pair introverted/extroverted (P¼0.002). Differences
between the ratings of the perspective drawings and the
model appear for the adjective pairs harmonious/discord
(P¼0.036), spacious/confined (P¼0.028), comfortable/
uncomfortable (P¼0.000) and static/dynamic (P¼
0.042).
ii. Next, factor analysis was performed in order to
observe which constructs (semantic scales) go together
and how these constructs are categorized (factorized)
when the medium that the colour scheme is presented in
differs. Table V shows the variance percentages
accounted for the factors and the orthogonal factor load-
ing matrices for seven adjectives of colour chips, abstract
graphics, perspective drawings, and 3D models. On the ta-
ble the four media arranged as columns. Classifications
given on top of them show the characteristics of the four
media (e.g., colour chips are presenting 2-dimensional,
abstract media where colours are loosely combined, while
abstract graphics are also 2-dimensional and abstract, but
colours are organized as compositions). On the rows the
categorization of the constructs are listed under the order
of extracted factors.
For the colour chips the first factor accounted for
28% of the variance, the second factor 26% and the third
factor 25%. The first factor gathers the adjective pairs
TABLE II. Corresponding Munsell notations and scheme/colour characteristics.
Schemes N1 N2 N3 N4 N5 HUE Value Chroma
1 5Y9/2 5Y9/6 2,5GY8/3 25GY3/1 7,5Y8/7 Similar-warm Contrasting Contrasting
2 10Y9/6 7,5R3/4 5YR6/14 5R4/12 10Y8/10 Similar-warm Contrasting Contrasting
3 7,5RP9/2 7,5R2/6 7,5R6/2 6,5R4/12 7,5RP4/8 Moderate Contrasting Contrasting
4 10B7/4 7,5GY8/4 5B7/4 5P7/6 7,5BG7/4 Contrasting Similar-light Similar-weak
5 9/0 5B5/1 5B9/2 2,5P4/4 7,5B5/8 Similar-cold Moderate Contrasting
6 5Y8,5/8 5Y8/10 3,75YR6/12 2,5PB7/8 2,5GY7/8 Contrasting Similar-light Similar-strong
7 5YR9/2 10R7/8 10B8/4 10R6/12 5PB5/10 Contrasting Similar-light Contrasting
8 5Y9/2 5P8/4 5YR9/6 5P7/6 5Y8/6 Contrasting Similar-light Similar-weak
9 7,5Y9/2 10B8/4 5GY8/6 2,5PB6/10 7,5Y9/4 Contrasting Similar-light Contrasting
10 10R9/2 10R8/4 10R6/6 7,5R10/4 10R5/8 Similar-warm Moderate Moderate
11 7,5BG8/2 7,5BG4/2 7,5B6/6 7,5BG4/4 2,5BG4/4 Similar-cold Moderate Similar-weak
12 7/0 10YR8/10 10R6/12 8,75R5/12 2,5Y8,5/12 Similar-warm Similar-light Contrasting
13 5B7/2 5B4/4 5P5/6 10GY4/6 10PB4/4 Contrasting Similar-dark Similar-strong
14 5/0 7,5P5/2 7,5P3/2 3,75R4/12 10RP2/6 Similar-warm Similar-dark Contrasting
N1, ceiling; N2, floor; N3, back and left walls; N4, right wall; N5, furniture.
TABLE III. Associations among colour chips, abstract graphics, perspective drawings, and 3d models over
seven semantic scales.
Chips/abstract
graphics
Chips/
perspective
drawings Chips/model
Abstract
graphics/
perspective
drawings
Abstract
graphics/
model
Perspective
drawings/
model
rPrPrPrPrPrP
Harmony 0.279 0.000** 0.141 0.005* 0.254 0.000** 0.494 0.000** 0.464 0.000** 0.440 0.000**
Pleasantness 0.049 0.339 0.000 0.997 0.053 0.296 0.310 0.000** 0.353 0.000** 0.371 0.000**
Comfort 0.161 0.001** 0.058 0.251 0.210 0.000** 0.339 0.000** 0.332 0.000** 0.375 0.000**
Spaciousness 0.239 0.000** 0.041 0.416 0.285 0.000** 0.405 0.000** 0.456 0.000** 0.399 0.000**
Dynamism 0.055 0.281 0.249 0.000** 0.221 0.000** 0.323 0.000** 0.283 0.000** 0.454 0.000**
Excitement 0.243 0.000** 0.120 0.018* 0.231 0.000** 0.361 0.000** 0.424 0.000** 0.304 0.000**
Introversion 0.116 0.022* 0.084 0.096 0.159 0.002* 0.365 0.000** 0.288 0.000** 0.358 0.000**
Volume 35, Number 5, October 2010 347
pleasing/unpleasing and harmonious/discord, and seems to
be ‘‘evaluation.’’ The second factor gathers the adjective
pairs static/dynamic, calming/exciting and introverted/
extroverted, and seems to be ‘‘activity.’’ The third factor
gathers the adjective pairs spacious/confined and comfort-
able/uncomfortable. For the abstract compositions, the
first factor accounted for 28% of the variance, the second
factor 26% and the third factor 25%. The first factor gath-
ers the adjective pairs static/dynamic, calming/exciting
and introverted/extroverted. The second factor gathers the
adjective pairs pleasing/unpleasing and harmonious/dis-
cord, and the third factor gathers the adjective pairs spa-
cious/confined and comfortable/uncomfortable.
The perspective drawings and the models showed
nearly the same order of factors and patterns of gathering
adjectives. Differences are observed only in the percen-
tages of the variances and in the order of two adjectives
in the second factor. For the perspective drawings, the
first factor accounted for 38% of the variance and the sec-
ond factor 30%. For the models, the first factor accounted
for 39% of the variance and the second factor 29%. The
first factor gathers the adjective pairs comfortable/uncom-
fortable, pleasing/unpleasing, harmonious/discord and spa-
cious/confined, and the second factor gathers the adjec-
tives static/dynamic, calming/exciting and introverted/
extroverted.
iii. Stepwise multiple regression was performed in
order to see whether the same colour and scheme charac-
teristics predict the semantic ratings of colour schemes
presented in different media. Table VI shows the predic-
tors of adjectives for each medium.
Results showed that different colour and scheme char-
acteristics are determinative over the ratings of adjectives
when the medium changes. In only one case (pleasing/
unpleasing presented in perspective drawings) are no sig-
nificant predictors observed. The only consistency is
between the predictors of ratings of the adjective pair
observed for introverted/extroverted. For this adjective
pair, Value was entered first and explained 6%, 8%, 15%,
and 7% of the variance in introversion for colour chips,
abstract graphics, perspective drawings and models,
respectively. Other characteristics explained a further per-
centage not greater than 2%. In 17 of the total 28 cases,
the explained percentage of variance (Rsquare change)
appeared at under 10%.
DISCUSSION
The present research was conducted to explore the archi-
tectural/spatial colouring process by investigating the con-
sistency of semantic ratings of four sequential stages of
the architectural colour design process, namely, colour
chips/samples, abstract compositions, perspective draw-
ings and 3D models. The results of the study have contri-
butions not only for previous research comparing colour
samples and applied colours in different fields of design,
but also for propounding a methodology for the architec-
tural colour design process.
From the findings it can be concluded that abstract media
can be used as a tool for initial colour-design decisions in
architecture, however, scrutinizing colour design through a
media simulating contextual characteristics and three-
dimensional relations is inevitable during the progression
of the architectural colour-design process. This work con-
tradicts the research indicating a strong correspondence
between colour chips and contextualized colours (e.g.,
Taft,
21
Sivik,
22
Hogg et al.
25
), the results showed that the
colour chips cannot be considered as efficient representa-
tives of architectural colouring. Significant associations
observed between the semantic ratings of abstract graphics,
and perspective drawings and models means that the
abstract graphics were evaluated more similarly with the
contextual media. Therefore, it can be said that abstract
compositions presenting both proportional relations and
adjacency of colours seem to be more appropriate tools
than colour chips.
There is an overall significant medium effect for six of
the seven adjectives: only for ‘excitement’ is no medium
effect observed (see the results of multiple comparisons
in ANOVA—Table IV). This observation may indicate
that this construct is evaluated independently from the
TABLE IV. Media effects on semantic ratings of seven adjectives (multiple comparisons in ANOVA).
FP
Scheffe’s range
Medium P
Harmony 3.28 0.020* Perspective drawing/model 0.036*
Pleasantness 3.81 0.010* Chips/perspective drawing 0.013*
Comfort 15.56 0.000** Chips/perspective drawing 0.000**
Chips/model 0.002*
Perspective drawing/model 0.000**
Spaciousness 10.80 0.000** Chips/perspective drawing 0.000**
Abstract graphic/perspective drawing 0.000**
Perspective drawing/model 0.028*
Dynamism 4.78 0.003* Chips/model 0.008*
Perspective drawing/model 0.042*
Excitement 0.70 0.550
Introversion 10.79 0.000** Chips/abstract graphic 0.045*
Abstract graphic/perspective drawing 0.000**
Abstract graphic/model 0.002*
348 COLOR research and application
TABLE V. The orthogonal factor loading matrices for seven adjectives of colour chips, abstract graphics, perspective drawings and 3D models.
2D Medium 3D Medium
Abstract Contextual
Combination Composition
Colour Chips Abstract Graphics Perspective Drawings 3D Models
Factor
1
EVALUATION
a
‘pleasure’
Pleasing–
unpleasing
Harmonious–
discord
0.88
b
0.86
b
28%
c
ACTIVITY
a
‘arousal’
Static–dynamic
Calming–exciting
Introverted–
extroverted
0.87
b
0.82
b
0.69
b
28%
c
EVALUATIONþ
ACTIVITYþ
POTENCY
a
‘agreeableness’
Comfortable–
uncomfortable
Pleasing–
unpleasing
Harmonious–
discord
Spacious–
confined
0.87
b
0.84
b
0.76
b
0.72
b
EVALUATIONþ
ACTIVITYþ
POTENCY
a
‘agreeableness’
Comfortable–
uncomfortable
Pleasing–
unpleasing
Harmonious–
discord
Spacious–
confined
0.87
b
0.82
b
0.79
b
0.72
b
38%
c
39%
c
Factor
2
ACTIVITY
a
‘arousal’
Static–dynamic
Calming–exciting
Introverted–
extroverted
0.84
b
0.82
b
0.63
b
26%
c
EVALUATION
a
‘pleasure’
Pleasing–
unpleasing
Harmonious–
discord
0.85
b
0.84
b
26%
c
ACTIVITY
a
‘arousal’
Static–
dynamic
Calming–
exciting
Introverted–
extroverted
0.88
b
0.79
b
0.69
b
30%
c
ACTIVITY
a
‘arousal’
Calming–
exciting
Static–dynamic
Introverted–
extroverted
0.84
b
0.82
b
0.66
b
29%
c
Factor
3
EVALUATION
þPOTENCY
a
‘estimation’
Spacious–
confined
Comfortable–
uncomfortable
0.87
b
0.77
b
EVALUATION
þPOTENCY
a
‘estimation’
Spacious–
confined
Comfortable–
uncomfortable
0.87
b
0.74
b
25%
c
25%
c
a
Factors by Osgood et al.
17
b
Orthogonal factor loadings for the adjectives.
c
Variance percentages accounted for the factors.
Volume 35, Number 5, October 2010 349
characteristics of the medium. According to Scheffe’s
range test, the medium effect mostly differentiates abstract
(chips and abstract graphics) and contextual (perspective
drawings and model) visualizations. It can therefore be
said that the subjects’ criteria for evaluating abstract
media differed from their criteria for contextual situations.
Between the perspective drawing and the three-dimen-
sional model, a significant medium effect was observed
for ‘spaciousness,’’ ‘comfort’and ‘dynamism.’This
finding may indicate that evaluations of these constructs
are more sensitive to a 3D appearance of the colour
scheme and/or architectural/spatial aspects.
Results of the factor analysis support the finding that
abstract media is being evaluated differently than contex-
tual situations. For the colour chips and the abstract com-
positions, ‘‘harmony’’ and ‘‘pleasure’’ extracted a factor
different than ‘‘spaciousness’’ and ‘‘comfort’’ did; how-
ever these four constructs extracted a single factor for
the perspective drawings and the models. The constructs
‘‘ harmony,’’ ‘pleasantness,’’ ‘spaciousness’and ‘comfort’
are accounted for as the first factor for contextualized media
(perspective drawing and model) and this factor seems to
be related to the ‘agreeableness/appropriateness’of the
colour schemes for the defined context. This indicates that
in an architectural/spatial context, constructs related to
architectural/spatial quality cannot be separated from ones
related to the sense of taste or the factor of evaluation
20
(pleasure
27
) (see the results of factor analysis—Table V).
This finding also supports previous research
23,28
by
revealing the distinctive nature of spatial colour. For
the abstract media, ‘‘spaciousness’’ and ‘‘comfort’’ are
accounted for as the last factor. Different from
‘harmony’’ and ‘‘pleasure,’’ these two constructs are
seemingly the determinants of architectural/spatial quality
and relevant to an ‘estimation’ for architectural/spatial
use of colours.
The extraction of ‘‘harmony’’ and ‘‘pleasure’’ for
always the same factor recalls the hypothesis by Ou and
Lou
9
: ‘‘there is a strong link between colour harmony and
the emotion pleasantness’’ and ‘‘a harmonious combina-
tion can always give viewers a pleasing impression.’’
However in consideration of the subject group (interior
architecture students), this finding cannot be generalized.
In all the four media, the constructs ‘dynamism,’
‘‘ excitement,’’ and ‘extroversion’ are always accounted
for together. This result indicates that the evaluations of
these constructs are related regardless of the media in
which the colour scheme is presented. They also seem to
be related to the factor of activity
20
(arousal
27
) or stimula-
tion (see the results of factor analysis—Table V). This
implies that activity ratings are largely independent from
the media and/or the spatial context.
TABLE VI. Stepwise multiple regression of predictors of adjectives (only significant predictors are included).
2D Medium 3D Medium
Abstract Contextual
Combination Composition
Colour chips Abstract graphics Perspective drawings Model
variable
Rsq
change
Sig.
of tvariable
Rsq
change Sig. of tvariable
Rsq
change Sig. of tvariable
Rsq
change Sig. of t
Harmony V 0.068 0.000 V 0.062 0.000 C 0.038 0.000 schH 0.060 0.000
C 0.058 0.000 H 0.048 0.001 C 0.051 0.000
schC 0.012 0.020 C 0.017 0.016 schC 0.015 0.012
Pleasantness V 0.075 0.000 V 0.076 0.000 schH 0.027 0.003
C 0.026 0.001 H 0.016 0.008 H 0.015 0.014
Comfort C 0.123 0.000 H 0.133 0.000 V 0.062 0.000 C 0.108 0.000
schC 0.024 0.001 C 0.012 0.020 schC 0.058 0.000 H 0.034 0.000
schH 0.042 0.000
H 0.011 0.025
Spaciousness C 0.110 0.000 H 0.139 0.000 V 0.298 0.000 V 0.129 0.000
H 0.042 0.000 V 0.094 0.000 schC 0.015 0.003 C 0.048 0.000
C 0.030 0.000 schH 0.015 0.004 H 0.020 0.004
schH 0.016 0.004
schC 0.016 0.007
Dynamism schC 0.063 0.000 C 0.051 0.000 V 0.135 0.000 V 0.069 0.000
V 0.029 0.003 schH 0.036 0.000 C 0.107 0.000 C 0.030 0.000
schH 0.015 0.011 V 0.028 0.001 schH 0.018 0.002 schH 0.024 0.002
schC 0.021 0.003
Excitement C 0.044 0.000 schH 0.084 0.000 C 0.149 0.000 schH 0.066 0.000
schH 0.032 0.000 C 0.057 0.000 V 0.038 0.000 V 0.047 0.000
schV 0.011 0.035 schH 0.015 0.007 schV 0.044 0.000
C 0.039 0.000
schC 0.009 0.034
Introversion V 0.060 0.000 V 0.082 0.000 V 0.154 0.000 V 0.066 0.000
schH 0.020 0.003 H 0.021 0.003
H 0.019 0.004 schH 0.017 0.007
C 0.018 0.005 schC 0.017 0.008
Colour characteristics; H: Hue; V: Value; C: Chroma.
Scheme characteristics; schH: similarity/contrast in Hue; schV: similarity/contrast in Value; schC: similarity/contrast in Chroma.
350 COLOR research and application
Results of the multiple regression analysis showed that
different colour and scheme characteristics are determina-
tive over the ratings of adjectives, however, the percen-
tages explain a maximum total of 30% in the variance of
evaluation of constructs. These results (given in Table VI)
indicate that colour-combination emotions, and media
effects on their evaluation have complex interrelations
and inner dynamics, which are not studied within the
scope of this research. Nevertheless, some rough interpre-
tations can be made about the general pattern. According
to the results, colour characteristics (dominant Hue, domi-
nant Value and dominant Chroma) seem to be determi-
nant for the evaluation of abstract media while scheme
characteristics (similarity or contrast in Hue, similarity or
contrast in Value and similarity or contrast in Chroma)
appear more effective for the evaluations of contextual
media (especially for 3D models). These observations
strengthen the difference between abstract and contextual-
ized media used as different visualization techniques in
the architectural colour design process.
The overall findings of this research have implications
for colour designers who work on architectural/spatial col-
ouring. In architecture, colour chips can be used for initial
colour design decisions, however during the colour design
process the initial decisions have to be reconsidered by
using other media simulating complex colour and colour-
space relations. Abstract compositions presenting both
proportional relations and adjacency of colours seem to
be more appropriate representatives of colour-design com-
pared to colour chips. Perspective drawings may serve to
visualize architectural/spatial configuration of colour-
design, but three dimensional appearance of colour com-
bination can only be observed on 3D models. Thus it can
be concluded that colour chips, abstract compositions,
perspective drawings and 3D models may be used as se-
quential media during the progression of the process.
Light is a very important integral of colour perception
and spatial evaluation. Lighting types (natural and artificial
light) and illumination levels do also affect evaluations of
colour schemes presented in 3D media.
29
Examining the
effects of different lighting conditions (e.g., different light-
ing qualities, quantities and patterns), working with other
simulation techniques (e.g., computerized media, 3D models
at larger or full-scale, and real-life environment) and differ-
ent subject groups (e.g., designers/architects and nondesign-
ers/architects) may offer different results that could be com-
pared with the ones of this study.
1. Green-Armytage P. The value of knowledge for colour design. Color
Res Appl 2006;31:253–269.
2. Lenclos JP. Living in colour. In: Porter T, Mikellides B, editors.
Colour for Architecture. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold; 1976.
p 73–77.
3. Pile JF. Color in Interior Design. New York: McGraw Hill; 1997.
4. U
¨nver R, O
¨ztu
¨rk LD. An example of facade colour design of mass
housing. Color Res Appl 2002;27:291–299.
5. Smith D. Environmental colouration and/or the design process. Color
Res Appl 2003;28:360–365.
6. Mikellides B. Conflicting experience of colour space. In: Simon JG,
editor. In: Prooceedings of the Forth IAPC Congress. Belgium: Lou-
vain La Neuve; 1979. Vol. 2.
7. Janssens J, Mikellides B. Color research in architectural education—
A cross-cultural explorative study, Color Res Appl 1998;23:328–
334.
8. Akbay S. An analysis of basic design students’ intuitive and analytic
attitudes in colour decisions (MFA dissertation). Bilkent University,
Ankara, Turkey, 2003.
9. Ou L-C, Lou MR. A colour harmony model for two-colour combina-
tions. Color Res Appl 2006;31:191–204.
10. Arnheim R. Art and Visual Perception. Berkeley: University of Cali-
fornia Press; 1974.
11. Birren F. Principles of Color. Pennsilvania: Schiffer Publ. Ltd; 1987.
12. Itten J. The Elements of Color. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold;
1970.
13. Doyle ME. Color Drawing. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold;
1981.
14. De Grandis L. Theory and Use of Color. New York: Prentice Hall-
Abrams; 1986.
15. Burchett KE. Color harmony. Color Res Appl 2002;27:38–31.
16. Ha
˚rd A, Sivik L. A theory of colours in combination—A descriptive
model related to the NCS color-order system. Color Res Appl
2001;26:4–28.
17. Nemcsics A. Experimental determination of laws of color harmony.
I. Harmony content of different scales with similar hue. Color Res
Appl 2007;32:477–488.
18. Ou L-C, Lou MR, Woodcock A, Wright A. A study of colour emo-
tion and colour preference. II. Colour emotions for two colour com-
binations. Color Res Appl 2004;29:292–298.
19. Ural SE. Color in Architecture: Effects of Color Dynamics on Color-
ing Artificial Environments (PhD dissertation). Karadeniz Technical
University, Trabzon, Turkey, 1995.
20. Osgood CE, Suci GJ, Tannenbaum PH. The Measurement of Mean-
ing. Urbana: University of Illinois Press; 1957.
21. Taft C. Color meaning and context: Comparison of semantic ratings
of colors on samples and objects. Color Res Appl 1997;22:40–50.
22. Sivik L. Color meaning and perceptual color dimensions: A study of
exterior colors. Go
¨teborg Psychol Rep 1974;4:1–24.
23. Billger M. Colour combination. Effects in experimental rooms. Color
Res Appl 1999;22:230–242.
24. Destefani LRG, Whitfield TWA. Esthetic decision-making: How do
people select colours for real settings? Color Res Appl 2008;33:55–60.
25. Hogg J, Goodman S, Porter T, Mikellides B, Preddy DE. Dimen-
sions and determinants of judgements of colour samples and a simu-
lated interior space by architects and non-architects. Br J Psychology
1979;70:231–242.
26. Stahre B, Ha
˚rleman M, Billger M. Colour Emotions in Larger and
Small Scale. In: Prooceedings of the AIC Interim Meeting: Colour
and Paints, Brazil, 2004.
27. Mehrabian A. Pleasure-arousal-dominance: A general framework for
describing and measuring individual differences in temperament.
Curr Psychol 1996;14:261–292.
28. Ha
˚rleman M, Werner I-B, Billger M. Significance of colour on
room character: Study on dominantly reddish and greenish colours
in north- and south-facing rooms. Colour Des Creativity 2007;1:1–
15.
29. Ural S, Olguntu
¨rk N, Yilmazer S. Qualitative and quantitative
evaluations of colour schemes on 2D graphics, 3D models and artifi-
cial lighting alternatives (in Turkish), 6. In: Ulusal Aydnlatma Kon-
gresi (6th National Lighting Congress), Istanbul, Tu
¨rkiye, 2006.
Volume 35, Number 5, October 2010 351
  • ... The findings of the studies on comparing abstract media to applied colour(s) in the different fields of design (e.g. Osgood CE et al., 1957;Sivik L, 1974;Hogg J et al., 1979;Ural SE, 1995;Taft C, 1997;Ural SE, and Yilmazer S, 2010) show that there are differences in the evaluations of colour(s), besides the similarities. ...
    Conference Paper
    Full-text available
    In colour design process, various colour visualization techniques are being used by designers. These techniques comprise a wide range of media/tools such as colour chips, coloured drawings, 3D models at different scales, computer aided simulations etc. Architectural space is a more complex context considering its 3-dimensional effects on appearance and perception of colours, under different lighting conditions. Hence, this study was conducted to explore spatial colour design evaluations of a full-scale experiment room representing a working environment, where 30 participants evaluated chromatic and achromatic colour schemes applied on surfaces under two different illumination levels (500 lux and 196 lux on the working surface), via semantic ratings. The colour schemes were also asked to be evaluated as colour chips and coloured perspective drawings. The significant differences were observed between evaluations of the colour chips and the full-scale setups for both illumination levels. Harmony and pleasure set apart under different illuminations of full-scale setups. However, the findings related with the colour attributes (hue, chroma, value) correspondent with; hue and chroma appear more determinative over the adjectives related with pleasantness and arousal, while value appears determinative over spaciousness. The findings of research also supported that, hue and chroma are more effective on evaluating spatial quality.
  • ... It is widely believed that, in urban environments buildings have to be in harmony with each other (Ünver & Dokuzer, 2002) Color theorists and prac- tioners showed great interest in laws of color harmony (Sivik & Hard, 1994;O'Connor, 2010). Theory and research showed that analogous and comple- mentary colors could produce color harmony. ...
    Article
    Preference for 'color combinations' have received remarkably little empirical attention and no study compared people's responses to 'abstract color combinations' and 'color combinations in urban settings'. This study aims to fill this gap and focuses on color combinations rather than isolated colors. 22 color compositions (11 abstract graphics + 11 simulated urban settings) were created. Color compositions included analogous and complementary hues, warm and cool hues, low (5 hues) and high (10 or 11 hues) diversity color compositions. 104 participant evaluated color compositions for (1) arousal, (2) naturalness, (3) relaxation and preference for various objects and settings including (4) clothing, (5) bathroom walls, (6) mall indoors, (7) restaurant indoors, (8) house indoors, (9) building exteriors and (10) any type of object, using a 7-point bipolar scale. The results showed that; (1) color compositions of abstract graphics and pictures of simulated urban settings were rated similarly for ratings of naturalness and preference for any type of object and setting, (2) low and high diversity color compositions were rated similarly for all scales except preference for house indoors, (3) analogous and complementary color compositions were rated similarly for all scales except preference for clothing, (4) warm and cool color compositions were rated similarly for all scales except preference for bathroom walls. The applied value of these results and areas for future research are discussed. © 2018, Istanbul Teknik Universitesi, Faculty of Architecture. All rights reserved.
  • Purpose: The purpose of this study is to investigate what people with different demographic characteristics such as age and gender expect from hotel room design and examine how design preferences affect purchase intent and desire to stay and word-of-mouth behavior. Design/methodology/approach: The study was based on a quasi-experimental design conducted on 762 participants. The manipulations of room color and design style were prepared using the 3D modeling software, while age and gender were self-reported variables. Findings: The results indicated that age and gender moderate the relationship between hotel guest satisfaction and room design style. Younger guests prefer contemporary design style, while older guests show equal satisfaction with traditional and contemporary styles. Male guests prefer rooms decorated in masculine colors, while women are equally satisfied with masculine or feminine color schemes. Research limitations/implications: This study was conducted as a hypothetical, computer-aided experimental scenario. A field experiment captured guests’ satisfaction with an experimental hotel room. A substantive cause–effect relationship between hotel room visual servicescape stimuli and satisfaction was established. Practical implications: Identifying design style and color preferences of a hotel target market is paramount for investment payoff and further supports the customization of hotel services. Originality/value: This is the first experimental study to manipulate color scheme and type of design in a hotel room and capture their effects on satisfaction and behavior of guests with different demographic characteristics.
  • Article
    Full-text available
    This paper sets out to consider the relationship between design education, architects’ colour knowledge, colour orientations and colour use in design practice. Specifically, a survey of 274 architects, architectural academics and postgraduates in Australia and Iran addressed the questions—is design education informing colour knowledge, and does colour education and knowledge inform an architect’s colour use in their designs? The findings suggest colour use in architecture has two chief influences: (1) Colour Orientation (architects’ general attitudes and prejudices towards colour use); and (2) Contextual Variables (the cultural and physical context of designs). The study shows that while the amount of colour education that architects receive has little role in informing their colourfulness orientations (i.e., how colourful they believe architecture in general should be), the greater an architect’s colour knowledge the more colourful their designs will be. The study suggests that the colourfulness of contemporary buildings is likely influenced more by levels of architects’ theoretical colour knowledge and by their personal preferences rather than by contextual influences such as the cultural and physical context of a design, the building function, or client directives.
  • Article
    Deciding a colour for a product is a significant task for designers to attract consumer attention and communicate brand messages. It requires an initial analysis that explores consumer expectations within the sector, and this information is then used to inform development of a product design. This article discusses the application of the product colour development process during the initial phase of product design. Using a case study approach, one particular product category—a dishwashing liquid product was selected based on the suggestion from a leading U.K. consumer goods manufacturing company that colour is a major design factor for this product category. In the first phase of the study, interviews and an online survey were carried out with consumers (to explore what elements are important when they purchase a washing-up liquid product). In the second phase of the study, a colour meaning experiment was conducted to explore possible colours for dishwashing liquid packaging using a semantic differential method. The results show that yellowish and bluish green colours evoke positive responses while saturated and dark green colours are perceived more negatively.
  • Article
    Color comprises both subjective and objective aspects within its contextual nature. Research on color design tends to explore this seemingly contradictory concerns from theoretical point of view, as well as architectural and design practice. The aim of this study was to observe subjective, intuitive or heuristic and objective, knowledge-based or analytical attitudes toward color in design education. In the study 84 introductory design students were surveyed progressively to understand their color decision criteria after completion of three 2-dimensional colored exercises, specific in terms of color education. Students' responses to open-ended questions were coded according to the 5 categories, under 2 decision making processes derived from the literature; heuristic approach: preferential and symbolic criteria, and analytic reasoning: formal, thematic, and systematic criteria. A distinction between associative and emotional aspects of symbolic criteria was also revealed by the data analysis. The findings showed a shift from heuristic responses to analytic reasoning, as expected. Additionally, it is also investigated that students not only used heuristic approaches but also analytical components (formal and systematic) of color decision making in varying degrees as well, even before any color subjects covered. Thematic color decisions became a major part of the students' design considerations upon completion of color subjects. The observed increase in the number of color criteria interrelated by the students' among almost all categories explicated a complex decision making process particularly in color design and education. These findings were expected to lead to some further understanding in color decision making in design.
  • Article
    Color in urban design has become an important issue, each city may present different colors which help to define and describe its architectural features. In the study, color in urban design with architectural setting is studied, façade colors are analyzed with a specific emphasis on the following research questions; “Can color schemes be designed in respect to color-emotion associations?” and “Are color-emotion associations affective while designing architectural setting-urban environment?.” Non-color experts, 170 people, from different European and non-European countries were asked to match the most appropriate adjectives with the given street views in accordance to their color schemes. In the first step, the effect of color is identified in relation to architectural environment-urban setting, second the relative effect of color is studied as a component of the material. A categorical specification on color cognition and linguistic level of representation is attempted. The results can be a starting point to highlight the importance of preparing color schemes in regard to color-emotion associations. Abstract color schemes may also provide us an idea about image setting, especially at design process stage. In the study, keywords are linked as environment-response pairs; such as quiet, calming, lively, exclusive, reserved, and natural. Human psychophysical structure such as “warm-cool,” “heavy-light” in regard to visualizing certain colors are evaluated and described in terms of building materials.
  • Conference Paper
    Full-text available
    Energy efficiency and visual comfort are current issues among the topics of lighting and lighting design. Both of these issues are multifaceted in itselves. Moreover, energy efficiency and visual comfort/quality have to be provided, together, at an optimum level in lighting design. This condition renders lighting design quite complicated and vital, particularly for working places. The studies on energy efficiency and visual quality; are generally concentrated on the quantitative parameters, the results of such studies concluded as specifications summarized in codes and/or standarts, spatial color design is mostly disregarded, and a color is reduced to its one dimension; namely, value/lightness/ brightness. Such an approach is perceived as something disconnected with the aesthetical considerations of architectural design and qualitative characteristic of a space. However, lighting and color design are always interrelated and coexist in aesthetical, functional and technical means. This paper aims to append color and color design matters to energy efficiency and visual comfort/quality issues. Main concern of the study is required/recommended reflection factors of surfaces in relation with the maintained illuminances. For the study architectural context is decided as office environments and the research is focused on private offices designed for single user in public facilities. Firstly, existing color schemes of six different offices critically analysed and also users are asked to evaluate their own offices accordingly. Analysis and interviews showed that, there are many objectionable features in all six offices, but the users seem more susceptive and responsive to the color design by means of their color preferences. Secondly, color schemes are converted to achromatic schemes and both are applied on 3D models. Then, subjects rated 3D models against semantic scales, under different lighting conditions. Findings showed that; there are significant associations for evaluative adjectives between the evaluations of different illumination levels, while differences observed for descriptive adjectives. Hue and chroma appear more determinative over the adjectives related with pleasantness and arosal, while value appears determative over spaciousness. The findings of research also indicate that, hue and chroma are more effective on evaluating spatial quality. Moreover results of this study may have implications for the use of achromatic schemes in initial stages of color design, for achieving a balance between energy efficiency and visual quality standardization.
  • Article
    Full-text available
    People׳s visual perception and recognition of urban colorscape tones change significantly in hazy weather. A psychological experiment is conducted in this study to investigate visual comfort related to commercial and residential buildings. Visual observations are performed on the tones of an urban colorscape during hazy weather and air pollution in Harbin, China. Fifty-eight color samples selected through an orthogonal method are evaluated through a Likert scale by 30 subjects in a laboratory setting. Statistical analysis is performed with the maximal information coefficient and R language. Experimental results show that the changing threshold values of color tones are related to the visual comfort levels of the subjects. The influence of the three factors of color tones on visual comfort level is relatively independent, and the effects of value and chroma contrast on color comfort level are greater than that of hue contrast in hazy weather. Furthermore, the comfort recognition threshold values of color tones in hazy weather are determined through data comparisons and analyses.
  • Article
    This study compares semantic ratings of colour samples (chips) with those of the same colours applied to a variety of objects. In total, 25 participants took part in the colour-meaning experiment, and assessed 54 images using five semantic scales. In Experiment 1, simplified images (coloured silhouettes) were used whereas in Experiment 2 real images were used. In this article, the terms “chip meaning” and “context meaning” are used for convenience. Chip meaning refers to the associated meanings when only isolated colour chips were evaluated while context meaning refers to colour meanings evaluated when colours were applied to a variety of product categories. Analyses were performed on the data for the two experiments individually. The results of Experiment 1 show relatively few significant differences (28%) between chip meaning and context meaning. However, differences were found for a number of colours, objects, and semantic scales i.e., red and black; hand wash and medicine; and masculine-feminine and elegant-vulgar. The results of Experiment 2 show more significant differences (43%) between chip meaning and context meaning. In summary, the context sometimes affects the colour meaning; however, the degree to which colour meanings are invariant to context is perhaps slightly surprising. © 2016 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 2016
  • Conference Paper
    Full-text available
    It is well known that a colour’s appearance can differ between a small colour chip and the same colour applied to a real room. The impression of the colour changes between these circumstances; e.g. on the chip it can be subdued, while it is perceived as striking in the room. In this paper, we compare the results of a colour chip study, Colour Emotion, to Hårleman's full-scale room study. In the first study, textile chips were viewed against a grey background in a viewing cabinet. In the other study, two rooms were painted in 12 hues in two different nuances: NCS 1010 and NCS 1030. They correspond well to the hue areas and to two of the nuance categories used in the chip study. Semantic scaling was used in both studies. The two studies show a distinct difference between words associated to colours of the same nuance and colour category. A clear pattern could be seen. In the room, the colours were perceived as more distinct, stronger and they arouse much stronger emotions. Generally, a colour chip had to be much more colourful to give comparable associations.
  • Article
    This article deals with the way colours in a room affect each other through contrast effects and reflections. The study presented is based on the visual observations of the colour appearance of three inherent colours in a sequence of experimental situations. My point of departure was a yellow, blue, and red room. The room had distinctly different colour appearances of the same inherent colours when observed at different locations. The effects of both contrasts and reflections were evident. However, reflection effects seemed to dominate the situation. In a study of small experimental rooms, different combinations of the same three inherent colours were systematically tested. Comparisons were made to examine the differences in colour appearance between monochromatic rooms and multicoloured rooms with varying colour schemes. The illumination was kept stable. In the cases studied, it was obvious how the colours in the two-coloured rooms became more alike or “neutralized each other.” The contrast strengthening between abutting areas on the same level had distinct significance for colour appearance and perception of space. However, not in such a way that the perceived colour differences became larger in the two-coloured room than between each monochromatic rooms. © 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Col Res Appl, 24, 230–242, 1999
  • Article
    Full-text available
    In this article we treat a study concerning room character due to colour appearance and spatial evaluation in different compass orientations. Rooms of the same colour, but observed in light from different compass orientation will appear differently; their identity colours will differ. The question now is if these circumstances are confined to colour impression or if they also cause a different room character. A study is made in Sweden in the Northern hemisphere in which two rooms, a north and a south-facing were used, Ninety subjects, evaluated and compared the experimental rooms, equal in all respects except quality of light in 118 empirical studies. The walls were coloured consecutively in six hues in two nuances; the inherent colours were six pinkish and five greenish, plus one yellowish and one bluish colour. Room character was described aided by semantic scales. Data were statistically processed using the SPSS 1 statistical program to analyse connections between spatial evaluation, inherent colour and compass orientation. NCS colour vocabulary was used. Finally, verbal descriptions with own words were used as a supplementary method to unfold nuances in response. The study show that differences in hue and nuance affect evaluation of room character. Subjects reacted distinctly different on pinkish and greenish rooms; they had separate colour connotations. Room compass orientation resulted in different evaluation through weakening or strengthening of associations linked to the colours by colour connotations.
  • Article
    The colours and architectural characteristics of building facades are the major factors affecting the general appearance of cities. When cities are examined from various perspectives, first impressions are obtained from the geometrical forms and facade colours of buildings. The facade colour arrangements should reflect the features of the region and buildings. In this context various features of natural and artificial environments such as plant life, water elements, climate, and historical texture should be examined, and a facade colour arrangement should be designed according to the examination results. In addition, the other factors effective in determining the colour and style of a building, such as social-cultural background of the society and traditional and natural building materials, should not be forgotten because in some regions traditional buildings with special construction styles, materials, and colours create a specific identity for the settlements and cities. The aims of this article are to elucidate the colour contrast, colour arrangement, and colour design stages of mass housing and to explain the colour design of Bizimkent Mass Housing, which was constructed in a new dwelling zone in Istanbul, Turkey, as an example of such an arrangement. © 2002 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 27, 291–299, 2002; Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com). DOI 10.1002/col.10068
  • Article
    This study investigates the knowledge of architectural students about perceptual and psycho-physiological aspects of color, color nomenclature, existing myths and beliefs, and how color is used in their everyday work in the studios. Data were collected mainly by means of questionnaires, to first- and final-year students in five schools of architecture: three in Sweden and two in the United Kingdom. The results were analyzed for each country and educational level, and comparisons made between them. The findings show there is a severe lack of knowledge about color research in both countries. Reading color literature and attending lectures or conferences was rare. However, most participants reported positive attitudes toward color research, especially studies of color perception and color preferences. Knowledge of the Natural Color System (NCS) and British Standard (BSI) was poor, and only a handful of the respondents knew the theoretical basis of the systems. Most of the students complained about the lack of coverage of the subject area in lectures, seminars, or studio work, with very little theory and only few practical exercises. Because students perceive color design as their own future responsibility and basic design education seems to be the main source of color information, the problem of this deficient knowledge should be seriously addressed by researchers and educators. The results suggest ways of improvement and list the most appropriate and preferred methods of communicating research on color. © 1998 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Col Res Appl, 23, 328–334, 1998
  • Article
    A feature of the empirical literature in colour psychology is that little attention has been given to how people in real settings engage in colour selection. Surprisingly, with rare exceptions, we know almost nothing about this. Besides its theoretical significance within psychology, this question has practical implications for industries where colour choice is important. A study is reported into that most ubiquitous of activities, selecting a paint colour for the home. This used a retrospective method supported by qualitative data analysis (NVivo) in which participants described their process of colour selection. The results indicate a process that is far from uniform, but one that consists of two essential stages. The first involves arriving at what we refer to as an affective specification of the qualities sought. Once constructed, this leads to the second stage of matching colour attributes to that specification. Selecting a wall colour emerges as a surprisingly complex process, and one that corresponds more closely to a search than to a simple affective choice. A parallel is drawn with decision research, particularly the field of naturalistic decision-making. From participants' reports, colour selection also emerges as a process that women appear to understand better than men. © 2007 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Col Res Appl, 33, 55–60, 2008
  • Article
    Colors rarely appear alone, they usually appear together. Since the number of colors is very large, the number of color combinations is almost infinite. Consequently, it is difficult to investigate how people perceive and evaluate color constellations in various contexts. It seems pointless to study arbitrarily chosen combinations. To bring order into the large number of possible color combinations, a structure is needed. The following article presents such a theoretical model—a theory of colors in combination. The article is based on the Natural Color System for the ordering of singular colors, which is in turn the practical extension of Hering's phenomenologically based Opponent Color Theory. Thus, the model is descriptive, i.e., the variables carry immediate meaning regarding the actual color appearance. Since the model is purely descriptive, it contains no information per se of whether colors are beautiful together or not. However, the model can be used as a reference structure to investigate the attributes and connotations of the experience of a given color combination (some examples of this are given). The most relevant attributes, or dimensions, of color combinations are categorized into three main groups, each with three subfactors: The Color Interval, with the subvariables Distinctness of Border, Interval Kind, and Interval Size, is the perceptual phenomenon that occurs in the transition from one color percept to another. The Color Chord, with subvariables Complexity, Chord Category, and Chord Type, expresses the character of the combination, how the colors “sound” together, i.e., the totality of the Color Gestalt. The Color Tuning, with subvariables Surface Relations, Color Relations, and Order Rhythm, refers to some of the different ways color combinations can be varied. The present color combination model should be seen as a theoretical, albeit empirically based, starting point for further studies of people's perception of color constellations, a scientific area that still, probably because of its complexity, seems to be uncharted territory. © 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Col Res Appl, 26, 4–28, 2001